Skip to main content
  • Primary research
  • Open access
  • Published:

Poor performance on the Iowa gambling task in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder

Abstract

Background

Several lines of evidence implicate orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in the pathophysiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). The purpose of this study was to investigate neuropsychological dysfunction of the orbitofrontal cortex in children with OCD.

Methods

The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), which reflects orbitofrontal cortex function, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), which is associated with functioning of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, were administered to 22 children with OCD and 22 healthy controls matched for gender, age, and intelligence.

Results

OCD patients displayed poor performance on the IGT. In contrast, performance on the WCST was not impaired in OCD patients compared to controls.

Conclusions

These findings are in line with previous studies demonstrating that OCD in childhood is associated with a dysfunction of orbitofrontal-striatal-thalamic circuitry.

Introduction

The current dominant model of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) focuses on abnormalities in cortico-striatal circuitry, with particular emphasis on the orbitofrontal-striatal-thalamic circuit [1, 2]. In particular, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), which facilitates behavioral flexibility upon negative feedback, plays a central role in most neurobiological models of OCD. For example, neuroimaging studies demonstrated that the activation of the lateral OFC along with several other cortical regions was abnormally reduced during a reversal learning task in OCD patients [3].

Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), which evaluates real-life decision-making abilities under conditions of ambiguity, is particularly sensitive to alterations in OFC function [4, 5]. In comparison, performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), which measures executive function or abstract reasoning abilities, is thought to be associated with functioning of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [6], a region not included in the orbitofrontal-striatal-thalamic circuitry model of OCD [7]. Previous studies have revealed impaired IGT performance in adult OCD patients [811], which may reflect OFC dysfunction in these individuals. However, to our knowledge, no study has investigated IGT performance in children with OCD. Recent findings suggest that gray matter and white matter changes in children and adolescents with OCD are broadly consistent with those identified in adult OCD patients [12, 13]. Therefore, we presumed that children with OCD might show impaired performance on the IGT, reflecting OFC dysfunction. Conversely, children with OCD may perform normally on the WCST if DLPFC function is not specifically associated with OCD in children.

In the present study, to test this hypothesis, we measured IGT and WCST performance in children with OCD and compared these results with those of gender-, age-, and intelligence-matched healthy, non-clinical controls. We excluded OCD patients comorbid with pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) because a comorbidity of these disorders is known to affect performance on neuropsychological tests [14, 15].

Methods

Subjects and clinical assessments

The study sample consisted of 22 Japanese OCD patients (between the age of 10 and 15 years). They were admitted as inpatients or outpatients at Kohnodai Hospital, National Center for Global Health and Medicine. Two trained child and adolescent psychiatrists made diagnoses following DSM-IV-TR [16] criteria. Participants with a current diagnosis of mood disorder, psychotic disorder, substance-related disorder, attention-deficit and disruptive behavior disorder, pervasive developmental disorder, or mental retardation were excluded from this study.

Normal healthy controls were recruited from 2 local public schools by word of mouth. A total of 47 students volunteered to participate, of which 2 were excluded due to obsessive symptoms, 1 due to tic disorder, and 3 due to pervasive developmental disorder. After matching for gender, age, handedness, and intelligence with the patient group, 22 were included in the analyses. Those with a current or past history of psychiatric diagnosis, psychiatric or psychological treatment, or absenteeism from school were excluded from the control group. A child psychiatrist conducted a 30-minute clinical interview with each control subject to rule out any current or past history of psychiatric disorders.

Assessment

Two trained child and adolescent psychiatrists assessed the severity of OCD symptoms using the NIMH-Global Obsessive Compulsive Scale (NIMH-OCS) [17] in both OCD patients and controls, and also via the Children Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (CY-BOCS) [18] in the OCD group. The handedness of each subjects was determined using the Hand Usage Questionnaire [19].

The Iowa Gambling task (IGT) [20] was administered by one trained child and adolescent psychiatrist following the procedures of the original version. Previous studies have used the IGT to assess decision making in children and adolescents with ADHD [21], psychopathic tendencies [22], and self-harm tendencies [23]. One difference from the original task was that play money was converted from U.S. dollars to Japanese yen [24]. The IGT requires subjects to repeatedly select cards (100 in total) from four decks of cards and subjects are instructed to maximize their profit. The aim of the game is to win as much money as possible, or, as far as possible, to avoid losing money. To achieve this, subjects must discover which are the most advantageous decks and preferentially select cards from those decks. Each time they turn over a card, subjects will win some money; however, on turning over each card they also will sometimes have to pay a penalty according to a pre-programmed schedule of reward and punishment. Gains and losses are different for each card selected from the four decks. Decks A and B are “disadvantageous” because whilst they receive 10000 yen, the penalties are also higher, so they cost more in the long run (these decks are also termed high-paying decks). Decks C and D, on the other hand, are “advantageous” because whilst they receive only 5000 yen, the penalties are also lower, resulting in an overall gain in the long run (and are therefore termed low-paying decks). Thus, successful task performance relies on sampling more from decks C and D than from decks A and B. In the present study, subjects were informed that they could get a special gift if their money on hand exceeded 300,000 yen at the end of the task. The 100 selections were divided into five blocks of 20 selections each, allowing us to verify changes in the pattern of selections throughout the course of the experiment. We registered the number of disadvantageous cards selected in total, and also in each block of 20 selections.

Four clinical psychologists individually administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition (WISC-III) and the Keio version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) [25]. WCST is a test for executive function or abstract reasoning involving working memory [6]. It is also a sensitive test of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex function [26]. In the WCST, categories achieved (CA), total errors (TE), and perseverative errors (PE) of Nelson were evaluated [27]. Categories achieved indicates the ability to change abstract categories, and perseverative errors represent the perseveration of preceding errors or the disturbance of inhibition of preceding incorrect responses.

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 20 for Mac. T-tests were used to compare continuous variables (i.e., age and IQ), and Mann–Whitney tests were used to compare ordinal variables. The results were considered significant at p < 0.05.

Ethical considerations

This study was designed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the National Center for Global Health and Medicine. All participants gave written informed assent and their parents gave written informed consent after the procedure had been explained to them.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics are summarized in Table 1. The patients had been prescribed the following medications: sertraline (6 patients), paroxetine (2), and clomipramine (2). The following comorbidities were observed in the patients: tic disorders (3), trichotillomania (3), stuttering (1), and somatization disorder (1). No significant differences were observed between patients and controls in terms of age, gender, full scale IQ, or handedness. OCD patients scored significantly higher on the NIMH-OCS and lower on performance IQ compared to the controls.

Table 1 Subject characteristics and performances on the WCST

In the last section of the IGT (selections 81–100), the number of disadvantageous card selections was significantly higher in the OCD group compared to the controls (shown in Table 2 and Figure 1). Regarding performance on the WCST, there were no significant differences in CA, TE, or PE between the OCD patients and the controls (shown in Table 1).

Table 2 The number of disadvantageous cards drawn in the Iowa Gambling Task
Figure 1
figure 1

Order of card selection from the 1st to the 100thtrial. The black and red lines show the mean number of disadvantageous cards drawn by OCD patients and controls, respectively, in each block of 20 cards. Horizontal bars indicate standard error of the means. Children with OCD chose more cards from the disadvantageous deck towards the end of the task. OCD, obsessive compulsive disorder; * Significantly higher compared to controls (Mann–Whitney test; P < 0.05).

We also examined the associations between clinical parameters and the neuropsychological data. The number of disadvantageous cards selected in the last block of the IGT did not correlate significantly with age at onset, duration of illness, or CY-BOCS score at the time of assessment in the OCD group, or with current age, verbal IQ, or performance IQ in either the OCD or the control group (Pearson’s correlation; all p > 0.05). However, the CY-BOCS score during the period with the most severe symptoms in children with OCD significantly and positively correlated with the number of disadvantageous cards selected in the last block (r = 0.55, p < 0.01). The number of disadvantageous cards selected in the last block was also significantly greater in patients on antidepressant medications compared to patients not taking an antidepressant (Mann-Whitney’s test, p < 0.05). Total number of errors in the WCST was negatively correlated with current age in OCD patients (r = −0.44, p < 0.05) and with performance IQ in the controls (r = −0.55, p < 0.01); however, no other significant correlations were observed between WCST scores and the clinical parameters (all p > 0.05).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report on IGT performance in children and adolescents with OCD. The observed poor IGT performance suggests impaired decision-making function in OCD patients. Previous studies have described poor IGT performance in adult OCD patients [811] and one study demonstrated impaired IGT performance in patients with prominent hoarding symptoms [28], both of which are in line with our current findings regarding childhood-onset OCD. However, Nielen et al. [29] did not find significant differences in IGT performance between OCD patients and healthy volunteers.

Electrophysiological recordings of the OFC have revealed that orbitofrontal neurons play an important role in processing the motivational value of rewarding outcomes [3032]. This fits very well with the fact that IGT performance is specifically sensitive to OFC activity. In the current study, children with OCD performed similar to controls in the early stages of the IGT, but selected more disadvantageous cards towards the end of the task. The persistent selection of cards from disadvantageous decks in IGT is a behavior also observed in patients with damage to the OFC [20] [33]. Taken together, OFC dysfunction may be associated with the pathophysiology of childhood OCD.

The present findings that IGT performance correlated significantly with the CY-BOCS score during the period with the most severe symptoms but not with the CY-BOCS score at the time of the assessment suggest that IGT performance may be associated with the potential severity of OCD rather than the state of present symptom exacerbation. We infer that the poor performance on IGT in antidepressant users may be because antidepressants were more likely to be chosen as a treatment for those with severe symptoms during the initial clinical assessment. However, an alternative explanation may be that the negative effects of antidepressants on cognitive performance result in poor IGT performance. Further studies are needed to elucidate what factors influence IGT performance in children with OCD.

The current study showed no significant differences in performance on the WCST between OCD patients and controls. WCST performance is specifically related to function of the DLPFC [6], a region that has no direct influence on the orbitofrontal-striatal-thalamic circuit. Therefore, it is plausible that WCST performance is not as impaired as IGT performance in OCD.

The only previous study which examined WCST performance in children with OCD [34] reported reduced WCST scores in children with OCD compared to healthy controls, with effect sizes of d = 1.24 for total errors and d = 1.30 for categories completed. Our sample size had 97% power to detect an effect size of 1.2 at an alpha level of 0.05 (calculated by G*Power 3.1.3 [35]). Therefore, our results indicate that any potential difference in WCST performance between OCD patients and controls in the current study was smaller than that derived by Shin et al. [34]. We infer that the current inconsistency with the results of Shin et al. may be because patients and controls were not matched for IQ in their study.

There are numerous studies on WCST performance in adult OCD patients; however, results are inconsistent across studies. Several groups reported a reduced performance in OCD patients [9, 3638], while others reported no differences compared with healthy controls [3942]. The inconsistencies across these studies may be attributable, in part, to the comorbidity of PDD or ADHD in some of the OCD patients. Children with high-functioning autistic disorder or ADHD display poor performance on the WCST [14]. To better understand and clarify these findings, any comorbidity with PDD or ADHD, the age of onset and duration of OCD, the intelligence of the subjects, and the age of assessment should all be taken into account.

There are several limitations to the current study. First of all, the number of participants was too small to draw definitive conclusions. Secondly, some children with OCD were on medication, which may have affected the results. Finally, we did not use neuroimaging methods such as fMRI to confirm our inference that the function of the OFC was associated with the results obtained in our study.

In summary, children with OCD displayed poor performance on IGT but did not differ on WCST scores compared to controls. These findings are in line with the hypothesis that OCD is associated with dysfunction of orbitofrontal-striatal-thalamic circuitry. However, large-scale studies combined with fMRI are needed to confirm the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of childhood OCD.

References

  1. Saxena S, Brody AL, Schwartz JM, Baxter LR: Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1998, 35: 26-37.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Graybiel AM, Rauch SL: Toward a neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuron. 2000, 28: 343-347. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00113-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, Aitken M, Craig K, Owen AM, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ: Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008, 321: 421-422. 10.1126/science.1154433.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW: Dissociation Of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1998, 18: 428-437.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Grant S, Contoreggi C, London ED: Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making. Neuropsychologia. 2000, 38: 1180-1187. 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00158-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Berman KF, Ostrem JL, Randolph C, Gold J, Goldberg TE, Coppola R, Carson RE, Herscovitch P, Weinberger DR: Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia. 1995, 33: 1027-1046. 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00035-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Cavallaro R, Cavedini P, Mistretta P, Bassi T, Angelone SM, Ubbiali A, Bellodi L: Basal-corticofrontal circuits in schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a controlled, double dissociation study. Biol Psychiatry. 2003, 54: 437-443. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01814-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. da Rocha FF, Alvarenga NB, Malloy-Diniz L, Correa H: Decision-making impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder as measured by the Iowa Gambling Task. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2011, 69: 642-647. 10.1590/S0004-282X2011000500013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Cavedini P, Zorzi C, Piccinni M, Cavallini MC, Bellodi L: Executive dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive patients and unaffected relatives: searching for a new intermediate phenotype. Biol Psychiatry. 2010, 67: 1178-1184. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Starcke K, Tuschen-Caffier B, Markowitsch H-J, Brand M: Dissociation of decisions in ambiguous and risky situations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2010, 175: 114-120. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Starcke K, Tuschen-Caffier B, Markowitsch H-J, Brand M: Skin conductance responses during decisions in ambiguous and risky situations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2009, 14: 199-216. 10.1080/13546800902996831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Jayarajan RN, Venkatasubramanian G, Viswanath B, Reddy YCJ, Srinath S, Vasudev MK, Chandrashekar CR: White matter abnormalities in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Depress Anxiety. 2012, 29: 780-788. 10.1002/da.21890.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Zarei M, Mataix-Cols D, Heyman I, Hough M, Doherty J, Burge L, Winmill L, Nijhawan S, Matthews PM, James A: Changes in gray matter volume and white matter microstructure in adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2011, 70: 1083-1090. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Tsuchiya E, Oki J, Yahara N, Fujieda K: Computerized version of the Wisconsin card sorting test in children with high-functioning autistic disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Dev. 2005, 27: 233-236. 10.1016/j.braindev.2004.06.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kado Y, Sanada S, Yanagihara M, Ogino T, Ohno S, Watanabe K, Nakano K, Morooka T, Oka M, Ohtsuka Y: Executive function in children with pervasive developmental disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder assessed by the Keio version of the Wisconsin card sorting test. Brain Dev. 2012, 34: 354-359. 10.1016/j.braindev.2011.08.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. DSM- APATFO: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. 2000, Washington DC: APA

    Google Scholar 

  17. Insel TR, Murphy DL, Cohen RM, Alterman I, Kilts C, Linnoila M: Obsessive-compulsive disorder. A double-blind trial of clomipramine and clorgyline. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983, 40: 605-612. 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.04390010015002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Scahill L, Riddle MA, McSwiggin-Hardin M, Ort SI, King RA, Goodman WK, Cicchetti D, Leckman JF: Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: reliability and validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997, 36: 844-852. 10.1097/00004583-199706000-00023.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP: The measurement of handedness. Brain Cogn. 1987, 6: 175-183. 10.1016/0278-2626(87)90118-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994, 50: 7-15. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Garon N, Moore C, Waschbusch DA: Decision making in children with ADHD only, ADHD-anxious/depressed, and control children using a child version of the Iowa Gambling Task. J Atten Disord. 2006, 9: 607-619. 10.1177/1087054705284501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gao Y, Baker LA, Raine A, Wu H, Bezdjian S: Brief Report: Interaction between social class and risky decision-making in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Adolesc. 2009, 32: 409-414. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.09.001.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Oldershaw A, Grima E, Jollant F, Richards C, Simic M, Taylor L, Schmidt U: Decision making and problem solving in adolescents who deliberately self-harm. Psychol Med. 2009, 39: 95-104. 10.1017/S0033291708003693.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Fukui H, Murai T, Fukuyama H, Hayashi T, Hanakawa T: Functional activity related to risk anticipation during performance of the Iowa Gambling Task. NeuroImage. 2005, 24: 253-259. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Igarashi K, Oguni H, Osawa M, Awaya Y, Kato M, Miura M, Kashima H: Wisconsin card sorting test in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Dev. 2002, 24: 174-178. 10.1016/S0387-7604(02)00024-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Nelson HE: A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 1976, 12: 313-324.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment. 1995, New York: Oxford University Press, USA, 3

    Google Scholar 

  28. Lawrence NS, Wooderson S, Mataix-Cols D, David R, Speckens A, Phillips ML: Decision making and set shifting impairments are associated with distinct symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology. 2006, 20: 409-419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Nielen MMA, Veltman DJ, de Jong R, Mulder G, den Boer JA: Decision making performance in obsessive compulsive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2002, 69: 257-260. 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00381-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Hikosaka K, Watanabe M: Long- and short-range reward expectancy in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2004, 19: 1046-1054. 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03120.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Tremblay L, Schultz W: Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature. 1999, 398: 704-708. 10.1038/19525.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Tremblay L, Schultz W: Reward-related neuronal activity during go-nogo task performance in primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2000, 83: 1864-1876.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Bechara A: Disturbances of emotion regulation after focal brain lesions. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2004, 62: 159-193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Shin M-S, Choi H, Kim H, Hwang J-W, Kim B-N, Cho S-C: A study of neuropsychological deficit in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2008, 23: 512-520. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A: G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007, 39: 175-191. 10.3758/BF03193146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Bannon S, Gonsalvez CJ, Croft RJ, Boyce PM: Executive functions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: state or trait deficits?. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006, 40: 1031-1038.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lucey JV, Costa DC, Busatto G, Pilowsky LS, Marks IM, Ell PJ, Kerwin RW: Caudate regional cerebral blood flow in obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder and healthy controls on single photon emission computerised tomography. Psychiatry Res. 1997, 74: 25-33. 10.1016/S0925-4927(96)03017-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Okasha A, Rafaat M, Mahallawy N, Nahas El G, Dawla El AS, Sayed M, Kholi El S: Cognitive dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2000, 101: 281-285.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Abbruzzese M, Ferri S, Scarone S: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: no evidence for involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Psychiatry Res. 1995, 58: 37-43. 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02670-R.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Gross-Isseroff R, Sasson Y, Voet H, Hendler T, Luca-Haimovici K, Kandel-Sussman H, Zohar J: Alternation learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1996, 39: 733-738. 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00179-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Moritz S, Birkner C, Kloss M, Jacobsen D, Fricke S, Böthern A, Hand I: Impact of comorbid depressive symptoms on neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2001, 110: 653-657.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Moritz S, Birkner C, Kloss M, Jahn H, Hand I, Haasen C, Krausz M: Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2002, 17: 477-483.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by a Research Grant for Nervous and Mental Disorders (20B-6) from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Masaki Kodaira.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MaK designed the study and wrote the manuscript. MaK, YI, HU, MU and KW recruited and screened the study participants. MaK and YI diagnosed the study participants. MaK administered the IGT. MaK undertook the statistical analysis. AO and KS supervised the data analysis and writing of the paper. MoK, NS, and DS gave critical comments on the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 3

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kodaira, M., Iwadare, Y., Ushijima, H. et al. Poor performance on the Iowa gambling task in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Ann Gen Psychiatry 11, 25 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859X-11-25

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859X-11-25

Keywords